Science hero-worship became deadly in 2020. Many political leaders unquestioningly supported what scientists claimed to be effective prevention measures. Over the course of the pandemic, people died believing the vaccines to be 100% safe and effective.

Science hero-worship is not new, of course. But it is more important than ever to understand why some people uncritically accept scientific explanations – and what can be done to create critical barriers to the blind acceptance of science.

In my soon to be written book “Science Hero-Worship: Why It Happens and 5 Things You Can Do About It,” I will offer ways for you to understand and combat the problem. Despite a lack of credentials in psychology, I still know that everyone is susceptible to forms of science hero-worship. Most importantly, I know there are solutions.

Here’s my advice on how to confront five psychological challenges that can lead to science hero-worship.

But before I go any further, I have a confession to make. There will be no book, and what you are reading here is very far from being an original article. What I am actually doing is taking an article you can find on The Conversation, titled “Science Denial: Why It Happens and 5 Things You Can Do About It”, and tweaking it ever so slightly to show just how easy it is to turn the weapon on the owner. Keeping that in mind, please continue reading but feel free to consult the original to see how little needed changing in order to do the old switcheroo. Anyway, back to ‘my’ article:

Challenge #1: Social identity

People are social beings and tend to align with those who hold similar beliefs and values. Social media amplify alliances. You’re likely to see more of what you already agree with and fewer alternative points of view. People live in information filter bubbles created by powerful algorithms. And when journalists share misinformation, you are more likely to believe it and share it. Misinformation multiplies and science hero-worship grows.

Action #1: Each person has multiple social identities. I talked with a climate change catastrophe believer and discovered he was also a grandparent. He opened up when thinking about his grandchildren’s future, and the conversation turned to economic concerns, caused by thoughts of Net Zero. Or maybe someone is unaware of the invalidity of the ONS data on vaccines because so are mothers in her child’s play group, but she is also a caring person, concerned about the potential dangers of vaccines to immunocompromised children.

I have found it effective to listen to others’ concerns and try to find common ground. Someone you connect with is more persuasive than those with whom you share less in common. When one identity is encouraging blind acceptance of the science, leverage a second identity to make a connection.

———————

Okay, I’m going to have to stop right here because there are such things as copyright laws, and the switcheroo was so ridiculously simple that I have probably overstepped them already. There are, of course, four more challenges listed in The Conversation article with four corresponding actions to be taken. You will just have to take my word for it that very little would need to be changed within the remainder of the article to switch it from being half-baked instruction on how to tackle ‘science denialism’ into half-baked instruction on how to tackle blind faith in what scientists say – or, more to the point, what journalists say about what scientists say. Specifically, we have:

Challenge #2: Mental Shortcuts

In which the authors claim that ‘science denialism’ stems from people not bothering to take their time to think seriously about the science, preferring instead to fall back on lazy System 1 thinking. This is, of course, referring to the concept of System 1 and System 2 thinking devised by Kahneman and Tversky, and popularised in their book, “Thinking, Fast and Slow”. It doesn’t seem to have occurred to the article’s authors that intuitive System 1 thinking and deliberative System 2 thinking are equally relevant to decision-making, and that we all take advantage of their complementary nature regardless of where we fall on the denialist/believer axis. In fact, the easiest way to avoid System 2 thinking is to fall back on trusting the guy in the white coat carrying the clipboard. Remember, the authors are both psychology professors and so they have no excuse for committing such a gaffe.

Challenge #3: Beliefs on how and what you know

In which the authors repeat the canard that the bewildering and rapidly changing advice given out by scientists during the Covid crisis was just science working at its best and that to think otherwise betrays a failure to understand how science is supposed to work. Science, the authors patiently point out, is not about certainties.

The rise of ‘science denialism’ is also blamed on an imagined cohort of fair-minded journalists who, by presenting both sides of arguments, are giving the false impression that there is still debate to be had in areas where the science has already been settled. Science, the authors patiently point out, is all about certainties, as exemplified by the well-known 97% consensus on climate change.

Challenge #4: Motivated reasoning

In which the authors, as you might expect, seem to think that ‘science denialists’ are uniquely guilty. As per the advice they gave against employing mental shortcuts, the ‘science denier’ is encouraged to read around a subject before drawing a conclusion. Damn it! That’s where I have been going wrong all these years.

Challenge #5: Emotions and attitudes

In which the authors warn against letting one’s emotions play too great a role in one’s decision-making. Once again, this is a trait that is supposedly unique to the ‘science denier’ and could not possibly play a role in one’s willingness to swallow uncritically everything that scientists say about a posited existential threat. After all, since when has fear been an emotion?

In summary, the article in The Conversation, is yet another piece of sceptic-bashing nonsense in which the authors extol the benefits of critical thinking whilst failing miserably to employ it themselves. The two professors concerned make much of the dangers of ‘living in information filter bubbles’ whilst giving every impression of occupying one of their own. They warn against mental shortcuts but certainly seem to have fallen under their spell when seeking to interpret the relevance of System 1 and System 2 thinking. They bemoan the failure of the general public to understand the true nature of the scientific enterprise, but I see very little evidence that they have any greater understanding, despite (or because of?) their academic background. They chastise those who engage in motivated reasoning but I suspect that their own reasoning is as motivated as it gets. And, whilst they lay claim to an understanding that emotion is important in decision-making, as long as it is not allowed to get out of hand, one wonders how strongly their own emotions were running when they saw fit to produce such an obviously partisan piece.

There is a serious debate to be had regarding the relationship between science and the general public, but one of the greatest polluters of that debate is a large group of academics who insist on pathologising scepticism, and yet seem incapable of offering any legitimate insights regarding genuinely distinctive traits. Whether it be major offenders such as Stephan Lewandowsky and John Cook, or relatively small time felons such as the individuals who wrote this article, all seem to be very much part of the problem whilst purporting to offer the solution. Sadly, therefore, I will not be rushing out to buy “Science Denial: Why It Happens and 5 Things You Can Do About It”. Instead, I will wait for the much more promising, “Psycho-babble Books: Why They Are Written and 5 Things You Should Prefer to Spend Your Money On”.

13 Comments

  1. I now have more time on my hands than I ever had. Yet I spend less time investigating alternatives to my own “scientific” beliefs. I thus cannot legitimately claim to be engaged in scientific reasoning, even from an arm-chair.

    When I taught at UEA I was proud of myself for keeping abreast of both sides of the climate debate. In fact it was a survival tactic. Without knowing the evolving climate dogma, one is unable to counter it. With students subjected to CRU influence, that was a necessary survival tactic and one that gave me some cred when discussing climate matters.

    Thus, although I have lost much (or all) of my climate cred, and no longer seek out a balance of opinions I can still appreciate their value.

    Liked by 1 person

  2. Yes, well, it’s been a long time since I’ve paid any attention to The Conversation. The articles are usually written in a smarmy patronising “look, this is a bit brainy for you, but we can talk slowly and use kindergarten words, so even though we know you’re dumb, you should be able to follow along” kind of editorial house style.

    And of course, part of this is that a large number of Conversation articles kick off with “eggspurtsay”.

    There’s a lot to take in about how the new cultural elite has become so vicious in consolidating its power across the entire knowledge sphere. It’s no surprise really that Conversation articles are telling its devout readers that any dissent from the Narrative is simply a mental illness.

    Liked by 1 person

  3. I particularly like action #3:

    Recognize that other people (or possibly even you) may be operating with misguided beliefs about science.

    Does that “or possibly even you” also apply to the authors, I wonder?

    Liked by 2 people

  4. Liked this bit – ” have found it effective to listen to others’ concerns and try to find common ground. Someone you connect with is more persuasive than those with whom you share less in common. When one identity is encouraging blind acceptance of the science, leverage a second identity to make a connection.”

    “find common ground” sounds a good idea

    Like

  5. How many scientists can you recognise from their portraits? I am ashamed to say I can only identify about a dozen.

    Liked by 1 person

  6. Alan,

    We should all be able to rattle them all off, but I’m afraid I can’t. The Rasputin figure on the bottom row has me stumped and I could only remember the lady centre-top as ‘that famous female NASA mathematician’. I had to look her up. Shame on me.

    Like

  7. Jit,

    >Mendeleev?

    I thought that as well but decided against it.

    But you are right as it turns out!

    Like

  8. good post – made me have a look at That Pathetic Conversation post (2yrs old).
    only plus for me were the comments below from Diane Merriam v Chris Crawford & this link –

    Liked by 1 person

  9. “Reproducibility trial: 246 biologists get different results from same data sets”

    This is a poke in the eye for science generally and ecology in particular.

    “There can be a tendency to treat individual papers’ findings as definitive,” says Hannah Fraser, an ecology meta researcher at the University of Melbourne in Australia and a co-author of the study. But the results show that “we really can’t be relying on any individual result or any individual study to tell us the whole story”.

    Given two datasets – one on inter-sibling competition in blue tits, and one on interspecific competition in Eucalyptus seedlings – all sorts of answers came back from the participants.

    Despite the wide range of results, none of the answers are wrong, Fraser says. Rather, the spread reflects factors such as participants’ training and how they set sample sizes.

    Everything was going great and then she had to go and slip into post-normal science. Each question has a true answer, or we can all go home.

    https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-023-03177-1

    Liked by 1 person

  10. Dfhunter,

    Thank you for that.

    I found the exchange of comments that the original article encouraged to be quite typical given the subject matter. However, I was most intrigued when a particular gentleman admitted to being anti-science before having seen the light and converted. One of the authors found this very interesting and encouraged him to say more so that we may all benefit from his experience. Of course, from that author’s perspective, she would just be indulging in confirmation bias. What she really needed to hear from was someone who used to think exactly like her but had changed their view. That would be more in keeping with a scientific mentality. Think of a thesis and then try to disprove it.

    Given a particular line of thinking, if one can reverse the conclusions that can be drawn from that line simply by selecting an alternative set of examples to illustrate it, then one should conclude that your line is not getting to the bottom of the issues. Your logic should not be able to prove ‘A’ and ‘Not A’. That’s what I was trying to demonstrate with this article. Their’s is a half-baked thesis.

    As for the problems with p-hacking and reproducibility, these are so well-known that one would have hoped that the authors would wish to say something about it. Instead, too much is made of the safeguards provided by science’s competitive structure. And it’s too glib just to talk about science not being about certainties, particularly when you go on to extol the virtues of scientific consensus. ‘Confidence’ can carry the modifier ‘false’ far more readily than other nouns.

    Liked by 1 person

  11. Dfhunter. Thank you for the stimulus to read the comments. Very stimulating indeed, everything from the origin of Covid 19 to plate tectonics. Found myself rooting for Chris Crawford much of the time.

    Like

  12. Late to the party. Great article, John. In Chapter 7 of my book, I point out the same error (in this case by Lew and crew) to understand that bias goes both ways. Generally speaking, in socially conflicted issues relating to science, I think items 2) to 4) largely follow the course set by item 1), working both in the direction of the original article and of course the opposite per your version (in other words, polarising along lines of cultural identity). In representative samples of publics, which is to say mainly representing people who don’t have the time or expertise to investigate such issues in any meaningful depth, where there is enough social data (say for creationism versus evolution in the US, or the climate change issue anywhere), then the relevant identities can be mapped. However, for many people such identities do not lead to wholly ‘for’ or wholly ‘against’ stances, but mixes in different degrees depending on the scenario (e.g. mostly ‘for’ climate catastrophism in a very emotive unconstrained scenario, and mostly ‘against’ in a very strong reality-constrained scenario). In the early half of the covid pandemic, I think herd instinct was a big player too, not a cultural identity thing, but as time went on and the original panic slowly subsided, the issue both altered yet was absorbed into the existing cultural conflicts.

    Liked by 1 person

Leave a comment

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.